Preface |
|
v | |
Panel of Reviewers |
|
vi | |
|
|
|
Is Pointing the Root of the Foot? Grounding the ``Prosodic Word'' as a Pointing Word |
|
|
3 | (7) |
|
|
|
The Subcortical Foundations of Grammaticalization |
|
|
10 | (8) |
|
|
Pragmatics and Theory of Mind: A Problem Exportable of the Origins of Language |
|
|
18 | (8) |
|
|
Two Neglected Factors in Language Evolution |
|
|
26 | (8) |
|
|
Expressing Second Order Semantics and the Emergence of Recursion |
|
|
34 | (8) |
|
|
Unravelling the Evolution of Language with Help from the Giant Water Bug, Natterjack Toad and Horned Lizard |
|
|
42 | (9) |
|
|
Linguistic Adaptations for Resolving Ambiguity |
|
|
51 | (8) |
|
|
|
Modelling Language Competition: Bilingualism and Complex Social Networks |
|
|
59 | (8) |
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
Language, the Torque and the Speciation Event |
|
|
67 | (8) |
|
|
The Emergence of Compositionality, Hierarchy and Recursion in Peer-to-Peer Interactions |
|
|
75 | (8) |
|
|
Causal Correlations between Genes and Linguistic Features: The Mechanism of Gradual Language Evolution |
|
|
83 | (8) |
|
|
Spontaneous Narrative Behaviour in Homo Sapiens: How Does It Benefit Speakers? |
|
|
91 | (8) |
|
|
What do Modern Behaviours in Homo Sapiens Imply for the Evolution of Language? |
|
|
99 | (8) |
|
|
The Origins of Preferred Argument Structure |
|
|
107 | (8) |
|
|
Long-Distance Dependencies are not Uniquely Human |
|
|
115 | (8) |
|
|
|
|
How Much Grammar Does It Take to Sail a Boat? (Or, What can Material Artifacts Tell Us about the Evolution of Language?) |
|
|
123 | (8) |
|
|
The Role of Cultural Transmission in Intention Sharing |
|
|
131 | (8) |
|
|
|
|
The Role of Naming Game in Social Structure |
|
|
139 | (8) |
|
|
|
Do Individuals Preferences Determine Case Marking Systems? |
|
|
147 | (8) |
|
|
What Impact Do Learning Biases have on Linguistic Structures? |
|
|
155 | (8) |
|
|
Reanalysis vs Metaphor: What Grammaticalisation CAN Tell Us about Language Evolution |
|
|
163 | (8) |
|
|
|
Seeking Compositionality in Holistic Proto-Language without Substructure: Do Counter-Examples Overwhelm the Fractionation Process? |
|
|
171 | (8) |
|
|
Unravelling Digital Infinity |
|
|
179 | (8) |
|
|
|
Language Scaffolding as a Condition for Growth in Linguistic Complexity |
|
|
187 | (8) |
|
|
|
|
The Emergence of a Lexicon by Prototype-Categorising Agents in a Structured Infinite World |
|
|
195 | (8) |
|
|
Evolutionary Framework for the Language Faculty |
|
|
203 | (8) |
|
|
|
Artificial Symbol Systems in Dolphins and Apes: Analogous Communicative Evolution? |
|
|
211 | (8) |
|
|
The Adaptiveness of Metacommunicative Interaction in a Foraging Environment |
|
|
219 | (8) |
|
|
|
On the Impact of Community Structure on Self-Organizing Lexical Networks |
|
|
227 | (8) |
|
|
A Crucial Step in the Evolution of Syntactic Complexity |
|
|
235 | (8) |
|
|
Evolution of the Global Organization of the Lexicon |
|
|
243 | (8) |
|
|
|
|
251 | (8) |
|
|
What Use is Half a Clause? |
|
|
259 | (8) |
|
|
The Formation, Generative Power, and Evolution of Toponyms: Grounding Vocabulary in a Cognitive Map |
|
|
267 | (8) |
|
|
|
|
|
|
On the Correct Application of Animal Signalling Theory to Human Communication |
|
|
275 | (8) |
|
|
Natural Selection for Communication Favours the Cultural Evolution of Linguistic Structure |
|
|
283 | (8) |
|
|
|
Syntax, a System of Efficient Growth |
|
|
291 | (8) |
|
|
Simple, but not too Simple: Learnability vs. Functionality in Language Evolution |
|
|
299 | (8) |
|
|
|
Kin Selection and Linguistic Complexity |
|
|
307 | (8) |
|
|
Regularity in Mappings Between Signals and Meanings |
|
|
315 | (8) |
|
|
|
Emergence of Sentence Types in Simulated Adaptive Agents |
|
|
323 | (8) |
|
|
|
|
|
Desperately Evolving Syntax |
|
|
331 | (7) |
|
|
Constraint-Based Compositional Semantics |
|
|
338 | (8) |
|
|
The Emergence of Semantic Roles in Fluid Construction Grammer |
|
|
346 | (8) |
|
|
Broadcast Transmission, Signal Secrecy and Gestural Primacy Hypothesis |
|
|
354 | (8) |
|
|
|
Self-Interested Agents can Bootstrap Symbolic Communication if They Punish Cheaters |
|
|
362 | (8) |
|
|
|
Coping with Combinatorial Uncertainty in Word Learning: A Flexible Usage-Based Model |
|
|
370 | (8) |
|
|
Removing `Mind-Reading' from the Iterated Learning Model |
|
|
378 | (8) |
|
|
|
How does Niche Construction in Learning Environment Trigger the Reverse Baldwin Effect? |
|
|
386 | (11) |
|
|
|
|
Coexisting Linguistic Conventions in Generalized Language Games |
|
|
397 | (2) |
|
|
|
|
|
Complex Systems Approach to Natural Categorization |
|
|
399 | (2) |
|
|
|
|
Regular Morphology as a Cultural Adaptation: Non-Uniform Frequency in an Experimental Iterated Learning Model |
|
|
401 | (2) |
|
|
|
|
Neural Dissociation between Vocal Production and Auditory Recognition Memory in Both Songbirds and Humans |
|
|
403 | (2) |
|
|
Discourse Without Symbols: Orangutans Communicate Strategically in Response to Recipient Understanding |
|
|
405 | (2) |
|
|
|
Taking Wittgenstein Seriously: Indicators of the Evolution of Language |
|
|
407 | (2) |
|
|
|
|
|
|
An Experiment Exploring Language Emergence: How to See the Invisible Hand and Why We Should |
|
|
409 | (2) |
|
|
The Syntax of Coordination and the Evolution of Syntax |
|
|
411 | (2) |
|
|
|
The Archaeology of Language Origin |
|
|
413 | (2) |
|
|
|
415 | (2) |
|
|
How Complex Syntax Could Be |
|
|
417 | (2) |
|
|
The Multiple Stages of Protolanguage |
|
|
419 | (2) |
|
|
A Human Model of Color Term Evolution |
|
|
421 | (2) |
|
|
|
|
Evolution of Song Culture in the Zebra Finch |
|
|
423 | (2) |
|
|
|
|
|
Iterated Language Learning in Children |
|
|
425 | (2) |
|
|
|
Gesture, Speech and Language |
|
|
427 | (2) |
|
|
Introducing the Units and Levels of Evolution Debate into Evolutionary Linguistics |
|
|
429 | (2) |
|
|
What can the Study of Handedness in Nonhuman Apes Tell Us about the Evolution of Language? |
|
|
431 | (2) |
|
|
Unidirectional Meaning Change with Metaphoric and Metonymic Inferencing |
|
|
433 | (2) |
|
|
|
Recent Adaptive Evolution of Human Genes Related to Hearing |
|
|
435 | (2) |
|
|
Inhibition and Language: A Pre-Condition for Symbolic Communicative Behaviour |
|
|
437 | (2) |
|
Carlos Hernandez-Sacristan |
|
|
Pragmatic Plasticity: A Pivotal Design Feature? |
|
|
439 | (2) |
|
|
Continuity between Non-Human Primates and Modern Humans? |
|
|
441 | (2) |
|
|
After all, a ``Leap'' is Necessary for the Emergence of Recursion in Human Language |
|
|
443 | (2) |
|
|
Labels and Recursion: From Adjunction-Syntax to Predicate-Argument Relations |
|
|
445 | (2) |
|
|
Iterated Learning with Selection: Convergence to Saturation |
|
|
447 | (2) |
|
|
A Reaction-Diffusion Approach to Modelling Language Competition |
|
|
449 | (2) |
|
|
|
Accent Over Race: The Role of Language in Guiding Children's Early Social Preferences |
|
|
451 | (2) |
|
|
|
|
|
Language, Culture and Biology: Does Language Evolve to be Passed on by Us. and Did Humans Evolve to Let that Happen? |
|
|
453 | (3) |
|
|
Three Issues in Modeling the Language Convergence Problem as a Multiagent Agreement Problem |
|
|
456 | (2) |
|
|
|
The Development of a Social Signal in Free-Ranging Chimpanzees |
|
|
458 | (2) |
|
|
|
Gestural Modes of Representation-A Multi-Disciplinary Approach |
|
|
460 | (2) |
|
|
|
|
|
Extracommunicative Functions of Language: Verbal Interference Causes Categorization Impairments |
|
|
462 | (2) |
|
|
Form-Meaning Compositionality Derives from Social and Conceptual Diversity |
|
|
464 | (2) |
|
|
|
|
466 | (2) |
|
|
Origins of Communication in Autonomous Robots |
|
|
468 | (2) |
|
|
|
Handedness for Gestural Communication and Non-Communicative Actions in Chimpanzees and Baboons: Implications for Language Origins |
|
|
470 | (2) |
|
|
|
|
|
|
The Evolution of Hypothetical Reasoning: Intelligibility or Reliability? |
|
|
472 | (2) |
|
|
Simulation of Creolization by Evolutionary Dynamics |
|
|
474 | (2) |
|
|
|
|
Evolution of Phonological Complexity: Loss of Species-Specific Bias Leads to more Generalized Learnability in a Species of Songbirds |
|
|
476 | (2) |
|
|
|
Referential Gestures in Chimpanzees in the Wild: Precursors to Symbolic Communication? |
|
|
478 | (2) |
|
|
|
Modeling Language Emergence by Way of Working Memory |
|
|
480 | (2) |
|
|
|
|
Mechanistic Language Circuits: What Can be Learned? What is Pre-Wired? |
|
|
482 | (3) |
|
|
Reflections on the Invention and Reinvention of the Primate Playback Experiment |
|
|
485 | (2) |
|
|
An Experimental Approach to the Role of Freerider Avoidance in the Development of Linguistic Diversity |
|
|
487 | (2) |
|
|
Prosody and Linguistic complexity in an Emerging Language |
|
|
489 | (2) |
|
|
|
|
|
|
Communication, cooperation and Coherence Putting Mathematical Models into Perspective |
|
|
491 | (2) |
|
|
|
A Numerosity-Based Alarm Call System in King Colobus Monkeys |
|
|
493 | (2) |
|
|
|
|
On There and Then: From Object Permanence to Displaced Reference |
|
|
495 | (2) |
|
|
Signalling Signalhood and the Emergence of Communication |
|
|
497 | (2) |
|
|
|
|
Wild Chimpanzees Modify the Structure of Victim Screams According to Audience Composition |
|
|
499 | (2) |
|
|
|
An Experimental Study on the Role of Language in the Emergenece and Maintenance of Human Cooperation |
|
|
501 | (2) |
|
|
|
Replicator Dynamics of Language Processing |
|
|
503 | (2) |
|
|
|
Syntactical and Prosodic Cues in Song Segmentation Learning by Bengalese Finches |
|
|
505 | (2) |
|
|
|
Why the Transition to Cumulative Symbolic Culture is Rare |
|
|
507 | (2) |
|
|
A Gradual Path to Hierarchical Phrase-Structure: Insights from Modeling and Corpus-Data |
|
|
509 | (2) |
|
Author Index |
|
511 | |