1 Overview |
|
1 | (78) |
|
|
1 | (2) |
|
1.2 Survey of the Ten Cue Categories |
|
|
3 | (10) |
|
1.2.1 The Lexical Cue Categories |
|
|
4 | (1) |
|
1.2.2 The Bodily Cue Categories |
|
|
5 | (2) |
|
1.2.3 The Collateral Cue Categories |
|
|
7 | (2) |
|
1.2.4 The Background Cue Categories |
|
|
9 | (3) |
|
1.2.5 The Temporal Cue Categories |
|
|
12 | (1) |
|
1.3 Illustrating the Cue Categories in Complex Interaction |
|
|
13 | (8) |
|
1.3.1 Interaction of Compatible Cues to a Speech-External Target |
|
|
14 | (3) |
|
1.3.2 Interaction of Compatible Cues to a Speech-Internal Target |
|
|
17 | (3) |
|
1.3.3 Interaction of Incompatible Cues to a Speech-External Target |
|
|
20 | (1) |
|
1.4 Characterizing the Trigger |
|
|
21 | (4) |
|
1.4.1 Features of the Trigger |
|
|
22 | (1) |
|
1.4.2 Distinguishing the Trigger from Other Linguistic Forms |
|
|
22 | (3) |
|
1.5 Characterizing the Cues |
|
|
25 | (14) |
|
|
26 | (8) |
|
|
34 | (5) |
|
1.6 Characterizing the Target |
|
|
39 | (17) |
|
1.6.1 The Open Ontology of Speech-Internal Targets |
|
|
40 | (11) |
|
1.6.2 The Open Ontology of Speech-External Targets |
|
|
51 | (5) |
|
1.7 Characterizing Space and Time |
|
|
56 | (8) |
|
1.7.1 The Conceptualization of Space |
|
|
56 | (2) |
|
1.7.2 The Corresponding Conceptualization of Time |
|
|
58 | (3) |
|
1.7.3 Properties Unique to Time |
|
|
61 | (3) |
|
1.8 Relating This Study to Others |
|
|
64 | (8) |
|
1.8.1 General Approach Differences |
|
|
64 | (2) |
|
1.8.2 Specific Approach Comparisons |
|
|
66 | (6) |
|
|
72 | (3) |
|
1.9.1 Reasons for Term Choices |
|
|
72 | (2) |
|
|
74 | (1) |
|
|
75 | (4) |
I Lexical Cue Categories |
|
79 | (124) |
|
|
83 | (74) |
|
2.1 Triggers as Complex Constructions |
|
|
83 | (5) |
|
|
83 | (1) |
|
2.1.2 Triggers as a Target-Structuring System |
|
|
84 | (1) |
|
2.1.3 The Formal Realizations of Triggers |
|
|
85 | (3) |
|
2.2 Intrinsic Target Properties Specified by Triggers |
|
|
88 | (25) |
|
|
89 | (8) |
|
|
97 | (2) |
|
|
99 | (3) |
|
|
102 | (1) |
|
|
103 | (1) |
|
|
104 | (1) |
|
2.2.7 Domainality (the Target as External or Internal to Speech) |
|
|
105 | (6) |
|
|
111 | (2) |
|
2.3 Contingent Target Properties Specified by Triggers |
|
|
113 | (40) |
|
|
113 | (32) |
|
2.3.2 Direction of Remove |
|
|
145 | (4) |
|
|
149 | (1) |
|
|
150 | (1) |
|
|
151 | (2) |
|
|
153 | (4) |
|
|
157 | (36) |
|
3.1 Non-Target Properties Specified by Triggers |
|
|
157 | (6) |
|
3.1.1 Trigger Requirements for Particular Cue Categories |
|
|
157 | (3) |
|
3.1.2 Trigger Indications of Grammatical Properties |
|
|
160 | (3) |
|
3.2 Properties Not Specified by Triggers |
|
|
163 | (4) |
|
3.2.1 Excluded, Even Though Related to Other Trigger Specifications |
|
|
163 | (2) |
|
3.2.2 Excluded, Even Though Specified by Other Cue Categories |
|
|
165 | (1) |
|
3.2.3 Excluded, Even Though Specified by Other Systems or Experientially Basic |
|
|
166 | (1) |
|
3.2.4 Principles for the Exclusion of Specifications? |
|
|
166 | (1) |
|
3.3 Processes that Triggers Are Engaged In |
|
|
167 | (11) |
|
3.3.1 Triggers Specifying Mutually Exclusive Properties within a Category |
|
|
167 | (3) |
|
3.3.2 Cue Codetermination |
|
|
170 | (1) |
|
|
171 | (1) |
|
3.3.4 Joint-Domainality Triggers |
|
|
172 | (4) |
|
3.3.5 Specification Conflict |
|
|
176 | (2) |
|
|
178 | (9) |
|
3.4.1 The Ground of a Path as the Target of an Ellipsized Trigger |
|
|
179 | (1) |
|
3.4.2 The Patient of an Action as the Target of an Ellipsized Trigger |
|
|
180 | (1) |
|
3.4.3 The "Whole" that Contains a "Part" as the Target of an Ellipsized Trigger |
|
|
181 | (4) |
|
3.4.4 The "Complex" that Includes a "Component" as the Target of an Ellipsized Trigger |
|
|
185 | (2) |
|
3.5 Representing Trigger Features |
|
|
187 | (2) |
|
|
189 | (4) |
|
4 Co-Form Cues to a Target |
|
|
193 | (10) |
|
4.1 Co-Forms Providing Literal-Semantic Cues to a Target |
|
|
194 | (4) |
|
4.1.1 Co-Form Cues to a Target's Identity |
|
|
195 | (1) |
|
4.1.2 Co-Form Cues to a Target's Non-Identity Characteristics |
|
|
196 | (2) |
|
4.2 Co-Forms Providing Immediate-Pragmatic Cues to a Target |
|
|
198 | (1) |
|
4.3 Co-Forms Providing Further-Knowledge Cues to a Target |
|
|
199 | (2) |
|
4.3.1 For a Speech-External Target |
|
|
199 | (1) |
|
4.3.2 For a Speech-Internal Target |
|
|
200 | (1) |
|
|
201 | (2) |
II Bodily Cue Categories |
|
203 | (138) |
|
5 Gestural Cues to a Target |
|
|
207 | (94) |
|
5.1 Foundations of the Analysis |
|
|
207 | (9) |
|
5.1.1 Distinguishing Classes of Gestures |
|
|
207 | (1) |
|
5.1.2 Comparing the Classes of Gestures |
|
|
208 | (3) |
|
5.1.3 Outward-Targeting Gestures and Fictivity |
|
|
211 | (4) |
|
|
215 | (1) |
|
5.2 Target-Intersecting Gestures |
|
|
216 | (36) |
|
|
217 | (3) |
|
5.2.2 Divergence from the Prototype Starting at the Emission Phase |
|
|
220 | (22) |
|
5.2.3 Divergence from the Prototype Starting during the Midcourse Phase |
|
|
242 | (1) |
|
5.2.4 Divergence from the Prototype at the Termination Phase |
|
|
243 | (5) |
|
5.2.5 Target Filling after Intersection |
|
|
248 | (4) |
|
5.3 Target-Enclosing Gestures |
|
|
252 | (10) |
|
5.3.1 Enclosing through Projection |
|
|
253 | (6) |
|
5.3.2 Enclosing through Radial Expansion |
|
|
259 | (3) |
|
5.4 Target-Pervading Gestures |
|
|
262 | (3) |
|
|
262 | (1) |
|
5.4.2 Penetrative Pervasion |
|
|
263 | (1) |
|
5.4.3 Suffusive Pervasion |
|
|
264 | (1) |
|
5.5 Target-Coprogressing Gestures |
|
|
265 | (7) |
|
5.5.1 The Articulator Is Coaxial with the Targeted Path |
|
|
266 | (4) |
|
5.5.2 The Articulator Is Oblique to the Targeted Path |
|
|
270 | (1) |
|
5.5.3 The Articulator Is Parallel but Not Coaxial with the Targeted Path |
|
|
271 | (1) |
|
5.6 Target-Paralleling Gestures |
|
|
272 | (4) |
|
5.6.1 One-Dimensional Paralleling |
|
|
272 | (2) |
|
5.6.2 Two-Dimensional Paralleling |
|
|
274 | (2) |
|
5.7 Target-Accessing Gestures |
|
|
276 | (2) |
|
5.8 Target-Beholding Gestures |
|
|
278 | (1) |
|
5.8.1 Eccetive Use of the Gesture |
|
|
279 | (1) |
|
5.9 Target-Neighboring Gestures |
|
|
279 | (1) |
|
5.10 Target-Contacting Gestures |
|
|
280 | (6) |
|
5.10.1 The Target Is Ontologically an Entity |
|
|
281 | (1) |
|
5.10.2 The Target Is Ontologically a Location |
|
|
282 | (4) |
|
5.11 Target-Affecting Gestures |
|
|
286 | (1) |
|
5.11.1 Target Maneuvering |
|
|
287 | (1) |
|
|
287 | (1) |
|
5.12 Degree of Engagement |
|
|
287 | (1) |
|
|
288 | (3) |
|
5.13.1 With Physical Targets |
|
|
289 | (1) |
|
5.13.2 With Locative Targets |
|
|
290 | (1) |
|
5.13.3 Distinguishing Degrees of Precision |
|
|
291 | (1) |
|
5.14 Gestural Cues to a Speech-Internal Target |
|
|
291 | (3) |
|
5.14.1 Cross-Speaker Targeting |
|
|
292 | (1) |
|
5.14.2 Single-Speaker Targeting |
|
|
293 | (1) |
|
5.15 A Cognitive System of Spatial Fictivity |
|
|
294 | (2) |
|
5.15.1 In Visual Perception |
|
|
295 | (1) |
|
|
295 | (1) |
|
|
296 | (1) |
|
|
296 | (5) |
|
6 Corporal Cues to a Target |
|
|
301 | (40) |
|
6.1 Two Corporally Based Regions |
|
|
302 | (1) |
|
6.2 The Figure's and Hearer's Relations to the Vicinal Region |
|
|
303 | (2) |
|
6.2.1 The Figure's Varied Relations to the Region |
|
|
303 | (2) |
|
6.2.2 The Hearer's Varied Relations to the Region |
|
|
305 | (1) |
|
6.3 Three Phases in Processing a Corporal Trigger |
|
|
305 | (10) |
|
6.3.1 Phase 1: Determining the Speaker's Location |
|
|
305 | (2) |
|
6.3.2 Phase 2: Determining the Region's Location |
|
|
307 | (6) |
|
6.3.3 Phase 3: Determining the Figure's Location |
|
|
313 | (2) |
|
6.4 Nonscalarity of the Vicinal Region |
|
|
315 | (4) |
|
6.4.1 Prepositions and Scalarity |
|
|
315 | (1) |
|
6.4.2 Corporal Here and Scalarity |
|
|
316 | (1) |
|
6.4.3 Path Specifiers and Scalarity |
|
|
317 | (2) |
|
|
319 | (1) |
|
6.6 The Corporal Cue Complemented by Other Exhibitive Cues |
|
|
320 | (7) |
|
6.6.1 With a Gestural Cue Added |
|
|
321 | (3) |
|
6.6.2 With a Hearer-Focus Cue Added |
|
|
324 | (1) |
|
6.6.3 With a Targetive Cue Added |
|
|
324 | (2) |
|
6.6.4 Proximal and Distal Reconsidered |
|
|
326 | (1) |
|
6.7 The Hearer's Location as the Corporal Cue |
|
|
327 | (3) |
|
6.7.1 Triggers Explicitly Requiring a Hearer-Centered Corporal Cue |
|
|
327 | (1) |
|
6.7.2 Triggers Implicitly Requiring a Hearer-Centered Corporal Cue |
|
|
328 | (1) |
|
6.7.3 Hearer Location and Salience |
|
|
329 | (1) |
|
6.7.4 Distance between Speaker and Hearer |
|
|
329 | (1) |
|
6.8 Paths toward/Not toward the Vicinal Region |
|
|
330 | (3) |
|
6.8.1 Motion toward but Not into a Vicinal Region |
|
|
331 | (1) |
|
6.8.2 Motion toward and into a Vicinal Region |
|
|
332 | (1) |
|
6.8.3 Motion toward a Noncurrent Vicinal Region |
|
|
332 | (1) |
|
6.8.4 Motion toward a Hearer's Vicinal Region |
|
|
333 | (1) |
|
6.9 Constraints on Corporally Based Regions |
|
|
333 | (1) |
|
6.10 Corporal Cues to a Speech-Internal Target |
|
|
334 | (5) |
|
6.10.1 A Target in the Narrative World |
|
|
335 | (1) |
|
6.10.2 A Target in Spatialized Discourse |
|
|
336 | (3) |
|
|
339 | (2) |
III Collateral Cue Categories |
|
341 | (62) |
|
7 Targetive Cues to a Target |
|
|
345 | (48) |
|
7.1 Targetive Feature Cues |
|
|
346 | (12) |
|
7.1.1 Felicity Requirements in Relation to Targetive Cues |
|
|
347 | (4) |
|
7.1.2 Effective Categories Guiding a Feature Search |
|
|
351 | (1) |
|
7.1.3 Combining Categories |
|
|
352 | (1) |
|
7.1.4 Unified versus Nested Feature Searches |
|
|
353 | (2) |
|
7.1.5 Type of Sensory Modality |
|
|
355 | (1) |
|
7.1.6 Size of Search Space |
|
|
356 | (2) |
|
7.2 Targetive Salience Cues |
|
|
358 | (10) |
|
7.2.1 Subeffective Categories Yielding to a Salience Search |
|
|
359 | (1) |
|
7.2.2 Salience-Associated Parameters and Their Relationships |
|
|
360 | (3) |
|
7.2.3 Illustrating the Parameters and Their Relationships |
|
|
363 | (4) |
|
7.2.4 Augmenting an Initial Salience Search |
|
|
367 | (1) |
|
7.3 Targetive Criterion Cues |
|
|
368 | (2) |
|
7.3.1 Criterion Categories Related to Effective and Subeffective Categories |
|
|
369 | (1) |
|
7.3.2 A Simplex Criterion Category |
|
|
369 | (1) |
|
7.3.3 A Compound Criterion Category |
|
|
369 | (1) |
|
7.4 Gestural Targetive Cues |
|
|
370 | (11) |
|
7.4.1 Parameters for Self-Targeting Gestures |
|
|
371 | (5) |
|
7.4.2 Type of Search for Gestural Targetive Cues |
|
|
376 | (2) |
|
|
378 | (3) |
|
7.5 Definiteness and Targetive Cues |
|
|
381 | (3) |
|
7.5.1 A Commonality between The and That |
|
|
381 | (1) |
|
7.5.2 Differences between The and That |
|
|
382 | (1) |
|
|
383 | (1) |
|
7.6 Targetive Cues to a Speech-Internal Target |
|
|
384 | (6) |
|
7.6.1 Speech-Internal Targetive Feature Cues |
|
|
385 | (2) |
|
7.6.2 Speech-Internal Targetive Salience Cues |
|
|
387 | (3) |
|
|
390 | (3) |
|
8 Hearer-Focus Cues to a Target |
|
|
393 | (10) |
|
8.1 Illustration of the Category |
|
|
393 | (2) |
|
8.1.1 In the Modality of Sight |
|
|
393 | (1) |
|
8.1.2 In the Modality of Hearing |
|
|
394 | (1) |
|
8.1.3 In the Modality of Smell |
|
|
394 | (1) |
|
8.2 Analysis of the Category |
|
|
395 | (5) |
|
8.2.1 The Three-Step Sequence |
|
|
395 | (4) |
|
8.2.2 A Further Perspective on Step 2 of the Sequence |
|
|
399 | (1) |
|
8.3 Hearer-Focus Cues to a Speech-Internal Target |
|
|
400 | (1) |
|
|
401 | (2) |
IV Background Cue Categories |
|
403 | (44) |
|
9 Environmental Cues to a Target |
|
|
407 | (24) |
|
9.1 Environmental Locating Cues |
|
|
408 | (10) |
|
9.1.1 Locating with Respect to a Subenvironment |
|
|
408 | (7) |
|
9.1.2 Locating with Respect to a Ground |
|
|
415 | (3) |
|
9.2 Environmental Bounding Cues |
|
|
418 | (3) |
|
9.2.1 Basic Environmental Bounding |
|
|
419 | (1) |
|
9.2.2 Divergences from the Basic |
|
|
420 | (1) |
|
9.3 Environmental Cues to a Speech-Internal Target |
|
|
421 | (7) |
|
9.3.1 Locating with Respect to a Speech-Internal Subenvironment |
|
|
422 | (4) |
|
9.3.2 Locating with Respect to a Speech-Internal Ground |
|
|
426 | (2) |
|
|
428 | (3) |
|
10 Epistemic Cues to a Target |
|
|
431 | (16) |
|
10.1 Knowledge about Perceived Nonlinguistic Phenomena |
|
|
432 | (1) |
|
10.2 Knowledge about Unperceived Nonlinguistic Phenomena |
|
|
433 | (4) |
|
10.2.1 The Unperceived Is Integrated with the Perceived |
|
|
433 | (1) |
|
10.2.2 The Unperceived Is Extrapolated from the Perceived |
|
|
434 | (1) |
|
10.2.3 The Unperceived Is Independent of the Perceived |
|
|
435 | (1) |
|
10.2.4 The Unperceived Is an Entity, Not a Location |
|
|
436 | (1) |
|
10.3 Knowledge about Linguistic Phenomena |
|
|
437 | (2) |
|
10.3.1 General Discourse Knowledge |
|
|
438 | (1) |
|
10.3.2 Local Discourse Knowledge |
|
|
438 | (1) |
|
10.4 Epistemic Cues to a Speech-Internal Target |
|
|
439 | (5) |
|
10.4.1 Knowledge about Nonlinguistic Phenomena for Speech-Internal Use |
|
|
439 | (3) |
|
10.4.2 Knowledge about Linguistic Phenomena for Speech-Internal Use |
|
|
442 | (2) |
|
|
444 | (3) |
V Temporal Cue Categories |
|
447 | (70) |
|
11 Chronal Cues to a Target |
|
|
451 | (36) |
|
11.1 Supersentential Chronal Interval |
|
|
452 | (18) |
|
11.1.1 The Figure's and Hearer's Relations to the Chronal Interval |
|
|
453 | (1) |
|
11.1.2 Three Phases in Processing a Chronal Trigger |
|
|
454 | (1) |
|
11.1.3 Phase 1: Determining the Trigger's Temporal Location |
|
|
454 | (1) |
|
11.1.4 Phase 2: Determining the Interval's Temporal Location |
|
|
455 | (3) |
|
11.1.5 Phase 3: Determining the Figure's Temporal Location |
|
|
458 | (6) |
|
11.1.6 Where Now and the Present Tense Differ |
|
|
464 | (2) |
|
11.1.7 The Nonscalarity of Chronal Now |
|
|
466 | (1) |
|
11.1.8 The Figure Event as Target |
|
|
467 | (1) |
|
11.1.9 Where Corporal and Chronal Targeting Are the Same or Differ |
|
|
467 | (3) |
|
11.2 Subsentential Chronal Interval |
|
|
470 | (8) |
|
11.2.1 Prosody in Subsentential Chronal Targeting |
|
|
470 | (3) |
|
11.2.2 Temporal Granularity |
|
|
473 | (1) |
|
11.2.3 Three Phases in Processing a Prosodic Chronal Trigger |
|
|
474 | (1) |
|
11.2.4 Temporal Co-Location |
|
|
475 | (1) |
|
11.2.5 Absence of Temporally Co-Located Components |
|
|
476 | (2) |
|
11.3 Cosentential Chronal Interval |
|
|
478 | (2) |
|
11.3.1 Cophrasal Chronal Interval |
|
|
479 | (1) |
|
11.4 Chronal Cues to a Speech-Internal Target |
|
|
480 | (3) |
|
11.4.1 Contraction of the Base Moment |
|
|
481 | (1) |
|
11.4.2 Expansion of the Base Moment |
|
|
481 | (1) |
|
11.4.3 Displacement from the Base Moment |
|
|
482 | (1) |
|
11.4.4 The Speech-Internal Continuum |
|
|
483 | (1) |
|
|
483 | (4) |
|
12 Perichronal Cues to a Target |
|
|
487 | (30) |
|
12.1 Perichronal Cues that Help Determine Other Cues |
|
|
487 | (20) |
|
12.1.1 The Basic Condition |
|
|
488 | (1) |
|
12.1.2 Cues Necessarily Concurrent with the Trigger |
|
|
489 | (5) |
|
12.1.3 Cues either Concurrent or Nonconcurrent with the Trigger |
|
|
494 | (13) |
|
12.2 Perichronal Cues that Help Determine the Target Directly |
|
|
507 | (3) |
|
12.2.1 Direct Perichronal Cues from Co-Forms |
|
|
507 | (2) |
|
12.2.2 Direct Perichronal Cues from Gestures |
|
|
509 | (1) |
|
12.3 Perichronal Cues to a Speech-Internal Target |
|
|
510 | (3) |
|
12.3.1 One Cue and One Trigger |
|
|
511 | (1) |
|
12.3.2 One Cue and Two Triggers |
|
|
511 | (1) |
|
12.3.3 Two Cues and Two Triggers |
|
|
512 | (1) |
|
12.3.4 Two Cues and One Trigger |
|
|
512 | (1) |
|
|
513 | (4) |
VI Sequences in Targeting |
|
517 | (68) |
|
13 Interaction Sequences and Joint Attention |
|
|
521 | (34) |
|
13.1 Analytic Framework for Interaction Sequences |
|
|
521 | (9) |
|
13.1.1 Narrowing Down to Interaction Sequences |
|
|
521 | (2) |
|
13.1.2 The Steps Are All Overt, All Verbal |
|
|
523 | (3) |
|
13.1.3 The Steps Are All Overt, Some Not Necessarily Verbal |
|
|
526 | (2) |
|
13.1.4 Some Steps Are Covert |
|
|
528 | (2) |
|
13.2 Triggers in a Basic Interaction Sequence |
|
|
530 | (4) |
|
|
531 | (1) |
|
|
532 | (1) |
|
|
533 | (1) |
|
13.2.4 Summary of the Targeting Interaction Sequence |
|
|
533 | (1) |
|
13.3 Taxonomy of Common Attention |
|
|
534 | (6) |
|
13.3.1 The Participation Parameter |
|
|
535 | (1) |
|
13.3.2 The Recognition Parameter |
|
|
535 | (1) |
|
13.3.3 The Elicitation Parameter |
|
|
536 | (1) |
|
13.3.4 The Epistemic Parameter |
|
|
537 | (1) |
|
13.3.5 The Four Parameters Conjoined |
|
|
538 | (1) |
|
13.3.6 Summarizing the Taxonomy of Common Attention |
|
|
538 | (2) |
|
13.4 Relating the Targeting Sequence to the Taxonomy of Attention |
|
|
540 | (2) |
|
13.5 Modified Targeting Sequences |
|
|
542 | (10) |
|
13.5.1 Hearer-Focus Cues Causing Modification |
|
|
542 | (1) |
|
13.5.2 Perceptual Ease Causing Modification |
|
|
543 | (3) |
|
13.5.3 Commonality Triggers Causing Modification |
|
|
546 | (2) |
|
13.5.4 Eccetive Triggers Causing Modification |
|
|
548 | (3) |
|
13.5.5 Offerative Triggers Causing Modification |
|
|
551 | (1) |
|
|
552 | (3) |
|
14 Cue Conflict and Its Resolution |
|
|
555 | (30) |
|
14.1 The Initial and Final Targets Have Similar Forms |
|
|
556 | (11) |
|
|
556 | (1) |
|
|
557 | (1) |
|
14.1.3 The Assessment Phase |
|
|
558 | (4) |
|
14.1.4 The Resolution Phase |
|
|
562 | (3) |
|
14.1.5 With Certain Factors Varied |
|
|
565 | (2) |
|
14.2 The Initial and Final Targets Have Dissimilar Forms |
|
|
567 | (4) |
|
14.2.1 The Reembodiment Operation |
|
|
569 | (1) |
|
14.2.2 With Certain Factors Varied |
|
|
570 | (1) |
|
14.3 The Initial Target Is Fictive |
|
|
571 | (7) |
|
14.3.1 The Final Target at Some Point Occupies the Indicated Location |
|
|
571 | (4) |
|
14.3.2 The Final Target Never Occupies the Indicated Location |
|
|
575 | (3) |
|
14.4 Resolution of Conflicting Cues to a Speech-Internal Target |
|
|
578 | (4) |
|
14.4.1 Conflict and Resolution in Tropes |
|
|
579 | (3) |
|
|
582 | (3) |
Glossary |
|
585 | (30) |
References |
|
615 | (8) |
Index |
|
623 | |